Sloan foundation “
“Ordered maps of the contralateral visua

Sloan foundation. “
“Ordered maps of the contralateral visual www.selleckchem.com/products/Y-27632.html field are presumed imperative for proper visual system function and are a core principle of the notion of hemispheric specialization (Huberman et al., 2008; Wandell et al., 2007). A prerequisite for this map formation in animals with binocular vision is a partially crossed projection of the optic nerves at the optic chiasm. Here, axons from the nasal and temporal retinae are guided by molecular markers to the contralateral and ipsilateral hemisphere, respectively (Petros et al., 2008). There they form a retinotopic map of the visual hemifield contralateral to the respective hemisphere

(Figure 1A and see Figure S1, available online). In congenital achiasma, this crossing is absent providing large-scale HDAC inhibitor erroneous input to the visual system (Apkarian et al., 1994, 1995; Victor et al., 2000; Williams et al., 1994). Both hemiretinae project to the ipsilateral hemisphere, which as a consequence receives input not only from the contralateral, but also from the ipsilateral visual hemifield. This poses a substantial challenge to the organization of visual field maps and prompts potential sensory conflicts. Despite these sizable aberrant projections, achiasmic humans have relatively normal visual function (Apkarian et al.,

1994, 1995; Prakash et al., 2010; Victor et al., 2000). Therefore achiasma offers a unique opportunity to study the principles governing cortical map development in humans. The knowledge of cortical mapping in this condition would provide insights into scope and mechanisms of developmental plasticity in the human visual system. The organization of the visual cortex and of the visual pathways beyond the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) in achiasma is unknown as only very few studies Urease addressed related issues (Victor et al., 2000; Williams et al., 1994). A study in a canine model of achiasma investigated the precise mapping of information in the visual system, but it was confined to the level of the

LGN. Here retinotopic maps of opposing hemifields in adjacent LGN layers were revealed (Williams et al., 1994). Another pioneering study addressed the cortical organization in human achiasma using functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) (Victor et al., 2000). This case study suggested that stimuli in opposing visual hemifields are represented in close cortical vicinity, but visual field map representations could not be reconstructed. To date the geniculostriate projections (LGN-striate or optic radiations), cortico-cortical projections and the corresponding cortical organization pattern are still obscure in achiasma, such that the developmental mechanisms that make the abnormal visual input available for visual perception remain unknown.

, 2011) However, the paucity of significant findings in very lar

, 2011). However, the paucity of significant findings in very large samples of cases and controls suggests that the

contribution of common genetic variants to heritability of BD is limited. Alternative INCB024360 cost approaches that focus on rare genetic variants are needed. One genetic approach that has been used effectively to overcome some of the problems of heterogeneity is the genome-wide analysis of rare copy-number variants (CNVs). Studies from our group (McCarthy et al., 2009, Sebat et al., 2007, Vacic et al., 2011 and Walsh et al., 2008) and from multiple independent groups (International Schizophrenia Consortium, 2008, Pinto et al., 2010, Stefansson et al., 2008 and Xu et al., 2008) have now firmly established that rare CNVs contribute Cobimetinib supplier to genetic risk for schizophrenia (SCZ) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and, in particular, that spontaneous (de novo) CNVs are important risk factors in the sporadic form of these disorders (Levy et al., 2011, Marshall et al., 2008, Sanders et al., 2011, Sebat et al., 2007 and Xu et al., 2008). Observations of a similar nature have been made in studies of BD. CNV loci at 16p11.2 (McCarthy et al., 2009) and 3q29 (Clayton-Smith et al., 2010, Mulle et al., 2010 and Quintero-Rivera et al., 2010) confer risk for multiple psychiatric disorders, and two studies have found preliminary evidence implicating both in BD (McCarthy et al., 2009 and Quintero-Rivera

et al., 2010). Two studies have demonstrated an enrichment of rare CNVs in patients with bipolar disorder (Priebe et al., 2011 and Zhang et al., 2009) as compared with healthy controls. In both studies, the greatest enrichment was observed in subjects with an earlier disease onset, defined as an age at onset (AAO) < 18 and < 21 in Zhang et al. (2009) and Priebe et al. (2011), respectively. However, two subsequent studies did not support these findings (Grozeva et al., 2010 and McQuillin et al., 2011). Thus, the role of copy-number variation in

conferring risk for bipolar disorder remains in question (Grozeva et al., 2010 and Zhang et al., 2009). Some of the earliest conclusive evidence for the role of rare CNVs in psychiatric disorders has come from family-based studies that examined the genomic burden of spontaneously occurring (de novo) CNVs (Marshall et al., 2008, Sebat et al., GPX6 2007 and Xu et al., 2008). De novo CNVs have consistently shown the strongest association with risk for autism (Itsara et al., 2010, Levy et al., 2011, Pinto et al., 2010 and Sanders et al., 2011) or schizophrenia (Xu et al., 2008), with a 5- to 10-fold enrichment in patients as compared with controls. We reasoned that if rare highly penetrant CNVs contribute to risk for bipolar disorder, the genetic effect would be most evident for de novo mutations. We further reasoned that the contribution of de novo CNVs to risk of bipolar disorder would be greatest in patients with an earlier disease onset (AAO ≤ 18).

foetus in the intestinal samples by yielding a sequence identical

foetus in the intestinal samples by yielding a sequence identical to corresponding published sequences only from T. foetus. Cat 3, a female Ragamuffin Ipilimumab order of four months of age, was subjected to necropsy with the tentative diagnosis of feline parvoviral infection. During the standard histopathological examination no evidence for a parvovirus infection was found. In the small intestine a mild crypt dilation and mild increase in mucosal

lamina propria lymphocytes, plasma cells, neutrophils and eosinophils was present. The large intestinal mucosa showed a mild crypt dilation with moderate amounts of mucus. The leukocytic infiltration was normal in the examined slides. With CISH of the small and large intestine using the OT probe scattered positively stained trichomonads were detected within the crypts (Fig. 1G). With the Tritri probe the same protozoal organisms were found to be positive (Fig. 1H). There were no positive signals with the Penta hom probe (Fig. 1I), suggesting the presence of

T. foetus. This was confirmed by PCR with the resulting amplicon having a nucleotide sequence that was 100% similar to corresponding regions of published T. foetus sequences. Several intestinal samples of cat 4, a female Persian cat of eight months of age, were submitted by a veterinarian due to enteritis completely resistant to therapeutic approaches. The microscopic examination of the colon showed a mild crypt dilation and mild increase of lamina propria neutrophils. In the gut lumen along the mucosal surface there were large amounts of parasite-like objects intermingled with large amounts of mucus, many neutrophils, some desquamated Tyrosine Kinase Inhibitor Library screening epithelial cells and erythrocytes and few eosinophils

and macrophages. The CISH with the OT probe showed positive staining of the parasite-like objects present in the gut lumen (Fig. 1J). The same picture was observed when the intestinal sample was analyzed using the Tritri probe (Fig. 1K). No positively stained parasites were found with the Penta hom probe (Fig. 1L), indicating the presence of T. foetus. This result could be confirmed by PCR and nucleotide Tolmetin sequencing producing a sequence identical to those published for T. foetus. In the small intestine no protozoa were detected by CISH although there were mildly increased lamina propria neutrophils, lymphocytes and plasma cells. Taken together, four of 102 cats were found positive for trichomonads. Thereof, three cats were found to be positive with the OT and the Tritri probe (two cats after necropsy, one organ submission) suggesting the presence of T. foetus, and in one cat (after necropsy) the OT and the Penta hom probe gave positive signals indicating the detection of P. hominis. In this study the suitability of the CISH technique for the detection of trichomonads in intestinal samples of cats was investigated. Three different CISH probes were shown to successfully detect trichomonads in cats. The OT (Mostegl et al., 2010) and the Tritri probe (Mostegl et al.

Our findings also provide evidence that the spatial layout of obj

Our findings also provide evidence that the spatial layout of objects in local contexts may be encoded upstream of the hippocampus in the POR rather than configured in the hippocampus. In addition to linking objects to places, available evidence suggests that the POR contributes to processing information about context by modulating attention to changes in the environment. In rodents, POR lesions alter performance in attentional orienting (Bucci and Burwell, 2004). The human PHC is also implicated in attention; activity in the parahippocampal place area attenuates for repeated scenes,

but only when the scenes were attended during initial and repeated presentations (Yi and Chun, 2005). In monkeys, neuronal activity in PHC is altered by changes in the context (Vidyasagar selleckchem et al., 1991) and by changes to stimuli in the periphery (Sato and Nakamura, 2003), suggesting a role in bottom-up, stimulus-driven attention. The rodent POR and primate PHC have anatomical connections with structures implicated in visuospatial attention,

including the pulvinar and the posterior parietal cortex (Broussard et al., 2006; Oleksiak et al., 2011; Posner and Petersen, 1990). The POR has robust reciprocal connections with the lateral posterior nucleus of the thalamus (Burwell et al., 1995). This structure is considered to be homologous to the primate pulvinar (Kamishina et al., selleck screening library 2009; Mason and Groos, 1981), which is strongly connected with the monkey PHC (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1985). Both the POR in the rat

and the PHC in the monkey are strongly interconnected with the posterior parietal cortex (Agster and Burwell, 2009; Burwell and Amaral, 1998a; Muñoz and Insausti, 2005; Suzuki and Amaral, 1994a). Interestingly, in monkeys performing a delayed match to sample task, activity in the posterior parietal cortex increased before activity in the medial temporal lobe increased (Saalmann et al., 2007). In the present study, the location selective cells in the POR exhibited selectivity CP-690550 chemical structure for the locations in which objects appeared, regardless of the identity of the object. Some cells even signaled location when the animal was viewing the location from a distance. These findings are consistent with an interpretation that POR signals attention directed to particular locations. Taken together, the evidence suggests that the POR, based on posterior parietal input, monitors the environmental context for changes and deploys attention to locations in which changes are likely to occur. A number of cells were selective for egocentric response to the left or to the right, regardless of the identity of the object or the side of the maze on which it was presented. The number of cells exhibiting this phenomenon was greater during the selection and reward epochs.

g , Duhamel et al , 1997) The predominance of neurons with eye-c

g., Duhamel et al., 1997). The predominance of neurons with eye-centered receptive fields lends support to the gain field model. A network using eye-position gain fields can be used to update visual information across saccades (Xing and Andersen, 2000). As noted above, when the eyes move between the Pifithrin-�� manufacturer presentation of the target and its capture by a saccade, there is a change in the retinal location of the target. In an encoding scheme using eye-centered neurons, the population of active neurons must change after each eye movement. This change,

the neural correlate of updating the retinal target location as a consequence of the eye movement, is referred to as “updating.” Xing and Andersen (2000) proposed an extension of the gain field model to perform updating. Briefly, postsaccadic eye position signals are combined with a stored gain field Decitabine mouse representation of the pre-saccadic target location to compute a second, updated gain field representation of the target location. The gain field representation can subsequently be read out to provide either head-centered or eye-centered target information. Gain fields thus provide a unified model for how spatial updating occurs as well as for how a distributed encoding of eye- and head-centered target location may be implemented. Despite the fact that gain fields

have been implicated in both reference frame transformations (Pouget and Snyder, 2000; Zipser and Andersen, 1988) and spatial updating (Xing and

Andersen, 2000), the evidence for their functional role is merely circumstantial. For example, neural network simulations confirm that gain fields are sufficient for computing supraretinal P-type ATPase target locations, indirectly supporting a role for gain fields in the computation of target location (Zipser and Andersen, 1988). Recent findings from PRR provide additional support for a computational role for gain fields. Chang et al. (2009) report a highly systematic arrangement—a strong negative correlation—between eye- and arm-position gain fields within individual PRR neurons, the presence of which they argue is difficult to explain away as an inconsequential contaminant or noise. They suggest that “compound” gain fields encode the distance between the fixation point and the hand. This distance is exactly the variable required to transform eye-centered visual target information into an arm-centered motor command for reaching. Nevertheless, direct evidence for a computational role of gain fields in neural circuits is difficult to obtain. Interventions to perturb or completely eliminate gain fields present technical challenges that are not easily overcome, and even worse, remain out of reach until we have a better grasp of the neural circuits and sensory inputs underlying gain fields. A major strength of the current study is that it proposes a more direct experimental test of the computational role of gain fields than has hitherto been performed.

(2003) suggested that the proportion of congenital infection decr

(2003) suggested that the proportion of congenital infection decreased with increasing parity of the mother, possibly due to increased immunity to transplacental infection with increasing age. Transient false-positive results, i.e. animals this website classified as N. caninum-negative with one or more isolated serological responses to N. caninum, were reported from the present study, in agreement with other studies ( Hietala and Thurmond, 1999, Chanlun et al., 2007 and Dijkstra et al., 2008). The low seropositive conversion rates found in this study are consistent with other longitudinal studies, in which

rates less than 8% were shown (Paré et al., 1998, Wouda et al., 1999 and Dijkstra et al., 2002a). The high seronegative conversion rates at Farms I and III are similar to results found by other studies (Waldner et al., 2001, Dijkstra et al., 2002b, Pfeiffer et al., 2002 and Moré et al., 2010). Studies on both mTOR inhibitor experimentally and naturally infected cattle have shown that the antibody levels can fluctuate, especially during gestation, and sometimes fall below the cutoff levels of the commonly used serological assays (Stenlund et al.,

1999, Guy et al., 2001 and Trees et al., 2002). This hypothesis may explain the return to seropositive condition in two of the three animals at Farm III that had seronegative conversion during the pregnancy period. However, Hietala and Thurmond (1999) reported that a few seropositive animals had a period of

negative samples, and this may have occurred in these three negative seroconverted animals. Although many studies have shown that N. caninum-seropositive cattle were more likely to be culled than were seronegative cattle ( Thurmond and Hietala, 1996, Waldner et al., 1998, Hobson et al., 2005 and Bartels et al., 2006), there was no significant difference in culling rate in the present study, between cattle that were N. caninum-seropositive and Moxisylyte seronegative, as previously reported ( Cramer et al., 2002, Pfeiffer et al., 2002 and Tiwari et al., 2005). This is the first longitudinal study on the seroprevalence of N. caninum in dairy herds in Brazil. The results confirm the importance of vertical transmission in the epidemiology of the parasite. Although there were indications for horizontal transmission, it does not appear to be the major route of N. caninum infection. High seronegative conversion was demonstrated at all the farms studied, and the culling rate of the animals was not associated with N. caninum infection. “
“The authors regret that the alpha value necessary to use the formula of Eq. (1) was incorrect. Page 303, Section 3.3, estimation of relative abundance, the second sentence should read as follows: Eq. (1) fitted the observed data perfectly for alpha = 0.992817 (Pearson coefficient of 0.999). The authors would like to apologise for any inconvenience caused.

This analysis revealed significant negative correlations between

This analysis revealed significant negative correlations between improvement rates in the car racing task and MD reduction in the left hippocampus (r = 0.49; p <

0.05) and right parahippocampus (r = 0.70; p < 0.005; Figure 2G). The improvement rate and starting performance (lap time in the first trial) were found to be highly correlated (r = 0.84; p < 0.001). Therefore, we performed partial correlation Selleck RG7420 between MD reduction and improvement rate controlling for the starting performance. In this analysis the parahippocampus showed significant correlation (r = 0.56; p < 0.05). Further analysis excluded the possibility that our observations (Figure 2) were derived from artifact bias caused by image preprocessing and the registration and normalization procedures (Supplemental Experimental Procedures; Figures S2B and S2C). This included overlaying our results on a single-subject FA map to verify that the effect does not include border regions between gray and white matter (Supplemental Experimental Procedures; Figure S2B). In addition, we verified the MD reduction in the hippocampus by region of interest analysis in the native space of each subject (Supplemental Experimental

Procedures; Figure S2C). To verify the statistical analysis (performed with parametric test), in addition to the paired t test, we performed the nonparametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test on the whole brain. This test is applicable if the distribution of the data is unknown, and is less sensitive to outliers than the paired t test. The same statistical threshold (p < 0.05, corrected) was used for both tests, Selleck EPZ5676 and both yielded similar results, namely a decrease in MD and an increase in FA in the same regions

(data not shown). To verify that the diffusion changes do not originate from volumetric or residual blood flow/activity traces, we performed voxel-based comparison of T1 and T2∗ maps (Supplemental Experimental Procedures) that were measured on the replication group. Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) analysis of the T1 scans before and after the task did not reveal any affected brain regions excluding anti-EGFR monoclonal antibody the possibility that the DTI observations are due to gross anatomical changes in the tissue. Voxel-based analysis (VBA) of the T2∗ maps before and after the task did not reveal any significant changes excluding the possibility that the DTI observations are due to changes in tissue susceptibility that may be caused by traces of neuronal function or blood vessel volume. The learning group was composed of young individuals of both genders. Behaviorally, no significant difference in improvement between the genders was obtained. However, it should not necessarily be inferred that the brain mechanisms that underlie the behavioral results were similar (Schweinsburg et al., 2005 and Speck et al., 2000).

These viruses were recovered from DNA, amplified, and concentrate

These viruses were recovered from DNA, amplified, and concentrated by ultracentrifugation. The titers to which

these viruses could be grown and concentrated were indistinguishable from the original SADΔG-GFP ( Table 1). Concentrated SADΔG-mCherry was tested in vivo by injection into the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN) of rats ( Figure 1A) or into two strains of Dinaciclib nmr glutamic acid decarboxylase (GAD)-GFP mice which express GFP in cortical interneurons (GIN) ( Oliva et al., 2000) in Figure S2A and G30 ( López-Bendito et al., 2004) in Figure S2B). As expected on the basis of previously published studies that used SADΔG-GFP injections into the thalamus ( Larsen et al., 2007), following 3 days survival, cortical neurons in locations known to project to the dLGN were infected and completely filled with mCherry, and this allowed clear visualization of detailed neuronal morphology, including dendritic spines and axons ( Figure 1A). Injections of SADΔG-mCherry selleck chemicals llc into dLGN of GAD-GFP mouse lines further demonstrated that infection was restricted from cortical inhibitory neurons that do not project to dLGN, and the GFP interneurons could be

clearly distinguished from the rabies-virus-infected, mCherry-expressing corticothalamic projection neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) ( Figures S2A and S2B). When we injected SADΔG-BFP into the rat V1, nearby neurons in layers 2/3 and 4 of the V1 were labeled with BFP ( Figure 1B). Similar results were observed for SADΔG-mCherry-Myc ( Figure S3). For many applications, it is important

to be able to mark infected neurons with one gene and to also express a second gene that, for example, controls cell function. This strategy is often preferable to the use of fusion proteins, because incorporation of a marker protein can prevent normal function, or in cases in which the gene product might be targeted to membrane compartments so that cell morphology is not easily visualized. Because ΔG rabies viruses expressing multiple exogenous genes have not been reported previously and the genome location of our inserts Meloxicam differed from that in previous two-gene viruses (McGettigan et al., 2003, Ohara et al., 2009 and Schnell et al., 2000), we were concerned that possible complications related to transcriptional regulation or viral capacity might prevent successful recovery and/or coexpression of both genes. We therefore sought to recover ΔG rabies virus encoding two different fluorescent proteins GFP and mCherry. This allowed direct visualization of whether both genes were reliably coexpressed. Two-gene expression was achieved by introduction of a second gene into an independent open reading frame in the SADΔG-GFP genome. The novel genes were introduced along with endogenous transcriptional control elements to take advantage of the rabies virus RNA-dependent RNA polymerase, which assures segmentation of separate genomic transcripts.

Similar to low doses of PTX, increasing γ oscillation after boost

Similar to low doses of PTX, increasing γ oscillation after boosting recurrent excitation with TBOA did not affect MC firing rate (+2.1 ± 1.6 Hz, p = 0.23 with paired t test, n = 8; Figure S3C). We next examined how pharmacologically increasing low-γ oscillations impacts the temporal properties of MC firing. MC autorhythmicity, as measured by the time of the first peak of the autocorrelogram, increased after drug injection (baseline, 15.5 ± 1.0 ms; PTX, 18.4 ± 0.7 ms,

p = 0.004, paired t test, n = 25; Figure 4E). A similar trend was observed on the interspike interval (ISI) distribution (Figure S3B). Selleck Natural Product Library Remarkably, the shift in MC autorhythmicity matched the pharmacologically induced

shift in the frequency of γ oscillations (mean γ oscillation period: baseline, 15.9 ± 0.4 ms and PTX, 18.4 ± 0.3 ms). http://www.selleckchem.com/products/dinaciclib-sch727965.html This change in rhythmicity was associated with a slight increase in the autocorrelogram amplitude (amplitude of the first peak normalized to the mean firing rate in baseline: 1.73 ± 0.05 and PTX: 2.03 ± 0.11; p = 0.017 with a paired t test, n = 25). Next, we examined the phase relationship between MC spiking and oscillations recorded with the same electrode. Under baseline conditions, all recorded MCs (n = 25/25) were significantly modulated by the γ oscillations (Rayleigh test, p < 10−7; Figure 4F). Spikes occurred preferentially in the descending

phase of the γ cycle (145.8° ± 5.6°). This phase preference also extended to low-γ and high-γ oscillations. Increasing low-γ oscillations did not impact the MC population phase preference (+3.9° ± 5.8°, p = 0.172 with a Hotelling paired GPX6 test, n = 25) but significantly increased the modulation strength (+48.2% ± 15.4%; Figure 4F). This was specific to the low-γ band since the high-γ regimes showed no change in modulation strength (Figure 4F). These effects were also observed after TBOA injection (Figure S3D). In addition to these modifications, MC spontaneous firing was slightly more irregular after drug injection, as measured by a modest increase in the ISI coefficient of variation (+4.7% ± 1.7%, p = 0.012 with a paired t test; Figure S3B). Although most cells were slightly modulated by the theta rhythm (24/25 cells, Rayleigh test, p < 0.005), the preferred theta phase of the MC population was widely distributed across the theta cycle (p > 0.1, Rayleigh test, n = 25). Nevertheless, drug treatment significantly increased the modulation strength of theta oscillations without significant changes in phase preference (Figure 4F). To characterize the spatial extent of γ oscillations, we measured the coherence of oscillations and MC spikes recorded from two sites spaced 400–500 μm apart (Figure 5A).

, 2004 and Vogiatzi et al , 2008) Interestingly, ubiquitination

, 2004 and Vogiatzi et al., 2008). Interestingly, ubiquitination by the ligase Nedd4 has also been shown to target synuclein for degradation in the lysosome, rather than by the proteasome (Tofaris et al., 2011). Although we do not know how changes in the expression of synuclein may actually influence the development of human PD, recent work has suggested that changes in clearance may promote degeneration. In particular, idiopathic PD has been found to associate

with mutations in the glucocerebrosidase (GBA1) gene. Mutations in GBA1 are responsible for Gaucher’s Y-27632 concentration disease, a recessive lysosomal storage disorder. However, the spectrum of phenotypes in Gaucher’s disease is very broad, with type 2 dying within the first 2 years of life and

type 1 surviving longer. Indeed, some of the so-called nonneuropathic type 1 patients eventually develop parkinsonism among other neurological MEK phosphorylation problems ( Alonso-Canovas et al., 2010 and Neudorfer et al., 1996). In addition, it has now become clear that heterozygotes with no overt symptoms of Gaucher’s disease develop PD at higher rates than controls. GBA1 mutations have been found in ∼7% patients with idiopathic PD, and up to ∼30% of Ashkenazi Jewish patients, with only 1.3% in the general population ( Sidransky and Lopez, 2012 and Sidransky et al., 2009). Mutations in GBA1 have also been reported in DLB but not MSA ( Farrer et al., 2009 and Segarane et al., 2009), supporting the difference in mechanism between MSA and Lewy related pathology. GBA1 mutations presumably increase susceptibility to PD by blocking the lysosomal degradation of α-synuclein ( Manning-Boğ et al., 2009), but it has been difficult to understand how a modest reduction in enzyme activity could impair lysosomal function enough to produce a degenerative disorder. Recent work has indeed suggested that α-synuclein accumulates

found in both a mouse model of Gaucher’s disease and induced pluripotent (iPS) cells from patients with Gaucher’s disease but attributed the increase to aggregation in the presence of increased membrane glucocerebroside ( Mazzulli et al., 2011). Consistent with the localization of synuclein to lipid rafts (which are enriched in sphingolipids such as glucocerebroside) ( Fortin et al., 2004), and its preference for particular lipid acyl chains as well as head groups ( Davidson et al., 1998 and Kubo et al., 2005), the mechanism by which GBA1 mutations confer susceptibility to PD may involve specific effects on cell membranes rather than a more general disturbance in lysosomal function that simply upregulates the normal protein. Indeed, much of the work on synuclein has focused on its misfolding and aggregation. Since many publications have addressed the pathways to misfolding of α-synuclein, including several recent reviews ( Breydo et al., 2012, Goedert et al., 2013 and Lashuel et al.